Natural selection increases female fitness by reversing the exaggeration of a male sexually selected trait
- Kensuke Okada,
- Masako Katsuki,
- Manmohan D. Sharma,
- Katsuya Kiyose,
- Tomokazu Seko,
- Yasukazu Okada,
- Alastair J. Wilson &
- David J. Hosken
- 1.
Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual Selection (Princeton University Press, 1994).
- 2.
Shuster, S. M. & Wade, M. J. Mating Systems and Strategies (Princeton University Press, 2003).
- 3.
Andersson, M. & Simmons, L. W. Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 296–302 (2006).
- 4.
Hosken, D. J. & House, C. M. Sexual selection. Curr. Biol. 21, R62–R65 (2011).
- 5.
Lande, R. Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 78, 3721–3725 (1981).
ADS MathSciNet CAS PubMed Article PubMed Central Google Scholar
- 6.
Kirkpatrick, M. Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution 36, 1–12 (1982).
- 7.
Fisher, R. A. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection (Claredon Press, 1930).
- 8.
Prum, R. O. The Lande-Kirkpatrick mechanism is the null model of evolution by intersexual selection: implications for meaning, honesty, and design in intersexual signals. Evolution 64, 3085–3100 (2010).
- 9.
Arnold, S. J. Mate Choice (ed. Bateson, P.) 67–108 (Cambridge University Press, 1983)
Abstract
Theory shows how sexual selection can exaggerate male traits beyond naturally selected optima and also how natural selection can ultimately halt trait elaboration. Empirical evidence supports this theory, but to our knowledge, there have been no experimental evolution studies directly testing this logic, and little examination of possible associated effects on female fitness. Here we use experimental evolution of replicate populations of broad-horned flour beetles to test for effects of sex-specific predation on an exaggerated sexually selected male trait (the mandibles), while also testing for effects on female lifetime reproductive success. We find that populations subjected to male-specific predation evolve smaller sexually selected mandibles and this indirectly increases female fitness, seemingly through intersexual genetic correlations we document. Predation solely on females has no effects. Our findings support fundamental theory, but also reveal unforseen outcomes—the indirect effect on females—when natural selection targets sex-limited sexually selected characters.
Introduction
Sexual selection typically acts more strongly on males and is responsible for the evolution of a vast array of exaggerated characters that enhance male sexual fitness components. Lande’s and Kirkpatrick’s models of sexual selection via the Fisher7 process—the null models of intersexual selection—clearly shows how this can occur. They also demonstrate how natural selection can oppose sexual selection as trait values move beyond their naturally selected optima . While theory is clear on the joint effects of natural and sexual selection on sexual trait evolution, explicit experimental tests of theoretical predictions are required to fully understand sexual trait evolution.
One potentially important source of natural selection that could affect the evolution of sexually selected traits is predation, and many studies have shown predation can seemingly oppose the exaggeration of male sexual characters. For example, sexual signals are conspicuous to potential mates but may also attract predators and parasitoid. This is particularly well documented in orthopterans and frogs, and this form of natural selection is probably responsible for the loss of cricket sexual signals on two Hawaiian isl. More generally, predation appears to reverse the evolution of extreme sexually selected phenotypes (reviewed in the ref. ) and males frequently reduce their sexual signaling in response to predation risk, which can result in decreased mating success when risk is high. Nonetheless, while there is ample evidence that predation selects against sexual trait enhancement, there is limited direct experimental verification that this selection causes evolutionary responses in these traits (but see e.g. ).
Female reproductive success can also be impacted by predation. For example, egg-carrying females can be slower and suffer higher predation rates. Resultant anti-predator behaviors may reduce foraging efficiency and reproductive activity, and thus be costly to females (reviewed in the ref.). Costs can accrue via delayed development, slower growth or postponed reproduction. Nonetheless, while there is ample evidence that predation selects on both females and males, the joint action of selection on the sexes is frequently investigated independently. Unfortunately, without exploring how predation affects both sexes, we are unlikely to fully understand how predation affects sexual trait evolution. This is especially true when intersexual genetic correlations link sexually selected male characters with female fitness, because selection on one sex can affect the other through these correlations. And again, controlled experimental tests for evolutionary responses to predation are usually not undertaken.
Here, we use the broad-horned flour beetle Gnatocerus cornutus to directly test for effects of predation on the evolution of a male sexually selected character (their mandibles) and female lifetime reproductive success (LRS). The enormously enlarged male mandibles are used in male–male fights, and males with larger mandibles have higher fighting and mating success. Females lack this exaggerated character completely. Previous work has shown that males with larger mandibles sire daughters with lower fecundity, and that directly selecting for increased (or decreased) mandible size results in decreased (or increased) female fitness (LRS). This apparently occurs because of the beetle’s genetic architecture, which means evolving larger mandibles results in the correlated evolution of masculinized females (even though females never develop mandibles). Basically, the enlarged male mandible requires a masculinized head and prothorax to function optimally and this means males with larger mandibles have smaller abdomens. Thus although females never develop mandibles, selecting on male mandibles ultimately affects female abdomen size, which likely determines the number of eggs a female carries.
These previous G. cornutus studies clearly document intralocus sexual conflict over beetle morphology and point to a negative intersexual correlation between (male) mandible size and female fitness, although this link has not been directly established and requires confirmation (c.f.). With this in mind we investigate the beetle’s intersexual genetic architecture for key focal traits using a standard pedigree-based mixed model approach (commonly referred to as an animal model). We also establish replicate (3/treatment) experimental evolution populations subjected to either male or female predation, along with control populations (n = 3, for nine total populations) to investigate how predation affects an exaggerate male trait and female fitness. The assassin bug Amphibolus venator, which preys on various stored-product insect pests including flour beetles, is the model predator. After eight generations of experimental evolution, we measure female fitness (LRS) and a range of morphological characters, including mandible size. Morphology was also measured during experimental evolution. We find strong effects of male-specific predation on morphology and female fitness, while predation on females alone had no effects. We additionally test for associations between male mandible size and predation likelihood and find larger mandibles result in greater likelihood of predation, and also show that evolving under predation made males less effective fighters.
Study of Nature and origin of "squirting" in female sexuality
Results
Genetic architecture
The genetic parameters estimated from the animal model analyses confirmed what previous experimental evolution studies had only inferred. Likelihood ratio comparisons of univariate models confirmed the presence of additive genetic variance in all four traits (Supplementary Tables 1–2) as expected. Note that the decision to treat the two mass measures (body/abdomen) as the same trait across the sexes was justified by an absence of significant genotype-by-sex (GxS) interactions in univariate models—a GxS implies sex limited genetic variance is present and can manifest as cross-sex genetic correlations (rGmf) <+1 and/or differences in sex specific additive genetic variances. Here rGmf was approximately +1 for both traits, but we do note there is a qualitative pattern of higher additive variance for body mass in females (see Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Multivariate models further confirmed the presence of additive genetic variance (LRT comparison of null model to one with a diagonal genetic matrix; χ24 = 74.96, P < 0.0001), as well as significant among-trait genetic correlation structure (LRT comparison of a model with a diagonal genetic matrix with all genetic correlations are fixed to zero to the full model in which all genetic correlations are estimated; χ26 = 39.02, P < 0.0001). Parameter estimates from the full multivariate model show substantial genetic variation in all traits measured and reasonably high trait heritability (Table 1). All traits were positively genetically correlated except male mandible size and female fitness (lifetime reproductive success) and male mandible size and abdominal mass, which were both strongly negatively correlation. Individual genetic correlations were nominally significant at α = 0.05 (based on |rG |> 1.96SE) except those between body and abdomen mass and between body mass and female LRS. The lack of correlation between body and abdomen mass is consistent with previous findings.
Predation also affected female LRS (Fig. 2d: F2,6 = 21.29; P = 0.002) (note females mated with “non-evolved” tester males). Post-hoc t-tests showed that this was because in the male predation treatment female fitness was higher (all P < 0.01), while the other treatments did not differ (control = female predation: P = 0.54). Thus exposing males to predators resulted in the evolution of smaller male mandibles and higher female fitness. As with males, female abdomen size also increased under male predation (Fig. 2e. Overall treatment effect: F2,6 = 10.75; P = 0.010): post-hoc t-test revealed the male predator exposure group evolved the largest female abdomens (all P ≤ 0.01: control = female predation P = 1.0)). Finally, female body size was unaffected by our experimental regimes (Fig. 2f. F2,6 = 1.65; P = 0.269).
Mandible size and predation likelihood
In the direct assessment of associations between mandible size and predation probability, 45 of 70 male G. cornutus were preyed by female A. venator within thirty minutes. The likelihood of being attacked by the predator was positively associated with mandible size (Fig. 3; Intercept (±se) = −7.18 ± 2.34; χ2 = 12.45; P < 0.001. Coefficient (±se) = 19.52 ± 5.89; χ2 = 15.09; P < 0.001). Possible reasons for this are discussed below.
Main resultsTo summarize the main findings: male-specific predation results in the evolution of an altered, more feminized male phenotype that includes reductions in the size of a male-limited sexually selected trait. Additionally, because of the beetle’s genetic architecture, the “new” demasculinized male phenotype is transmitted through to the female phenotype and results in higher fitness females (Fig. 4).
Discussion
Predation is frequently invoked as an evolutionary brake on the exaggeration of sexually selected traits and there are many studies consistent with this logic, usually documenting selection against larger characters or assessing macro-evolutionary patterns consistent with it we employ experimental evolution and directly demonstrate that male-specific predation not only reversed the exaggeration of a sexually selected trait, but additionally, this reversal results in higher female fitness. This boost to female fitness occurs because predators select against larger mandibles, and this results in a less masculinized beetle phenotype (including a larger abdomen), and because of shared genetic architecture across the sexes, females also become more feminized and produce more offspring. We discuss these findings in turn.
The likelihood of predation increases with male mandible size and when populations were forced to evolve with predation on males, smaller mandibles evolve. Thus increased natural selection via predation reduces the size of a sexually selected trait, which is broadly consistent with theory (reviewed in the ref. 1). Previous work with G. cornutus suggests why predation has greater impact on males with larger mandibles. Mandible size is negatively phenotypically and genetically associated with locomotor activity, and locomotor activity (running) is a predator escape mechanism in flour beetles. Reduced running and lower escape rates for males with large mandibles would explain the microevolutionary pattern we detect. Interestingly, there is no intersexual correlation for locomotion in this beetle49, so there was no expectation that predation on females should impact male running speed and hence morphology. Be that as it may, we clearly show that predation risk increases with mandible size and that this natural selection reverses the evolutionary exaggeration of a sexually selected male trait.
Females from the male-predation populations evolve higher fitness (higher LRS when mating with stock tester males) even though females were not directly exposed to predators. Previously Harano et al. demonstrated that directly artificially selecting for larger (male) mandibles reduced female fitness. This occurred because selection for increased mandible size resulted in a more masculinized phenotype, and Harano et al. suggested this masculinization rippled through inferred intersexually-shared genetic architecture to increase the masculinization of the female phenotype, thereby reducing female fitness. The intersexual genetic associations we document here, especially the negative male mandible-female LRS correlation, flesh out this explanation and confirm previous inference. Negative intersexual fitness associations are common because alleles conferring high fitness to one sex frequently lower fitness in the other. In addition to any sexually antagonistic selection on body morphology, male beetles with larger mandibles are also more aggressive toward females. Thus exposure to males with larger mandibles potentially reduces female LRS due to (misdirected) male attacks. Therefore, there could be two avenues to increased female fitness when predators select against large male mandibles, a reduction in ontogenetic conflict load (intralocus sexual conflict load), and reduced male-to-female aggression. In any case, predation on males causes an evolutionary reduction in mandible size and this results in higher female fitness. Thus male-biased predation indirectly selects for an increase in female quality, which is an interesting outcome.
The net population level effects of sexual selection and sexual conflict over optimal phenotypes are not clear, with for example, evidence that sexual selection can both increase and decrease population extinction rate. Additionally, intralocus sexual conflict (as documented in the flour beetle) is thought to constrain population adaptation because sexually antagonistic selection keeps each sex from its fitness optima. Our results suggest that predator removal of males with the largest sexual traits reduces intralocus (ontogenetic) sexual conflict costs, enhancing female reproductive performance, which should (all else being equal) increase population productivity. Relaxing other sexual conflict can also increase population fitness. It is interesting to note that predators are usually seen as suppressing prey populations, and this can have indirect benefits for ecological competitors of the prey taxon (predation on one species can open up ecological space and lead to higher fitness of the prey’s competitor taxa). As we show, sex-biased predation can have analogous indirect effects intra-specifically (predation on males increases female fitness). However, the indirect impacts we document are unidirectional since female-biased predation does not alter female fitness or male sexual-trait size.
Micro-evolutionary responses to our treatments were not readily apparent until about four or five generations of selection, after which there was clear treatment-specific divergence. Non-linear responses like this are common in selection studies (and see e.g., ) and there are many potential genetic and environmental mechanisms that can generate non-linearity. This includes genetic asymmetries (i.e., allele frequencies being unequal), differentially skewed genetic and environmental variation, non-additive genetic variation, and indirect selection. We have no way of knowing the mechanistic cause of the patterns we document, but note that responses to directly selecting on mandible size previously revealed similar non-linearity. There have also been many previous studies showing that males with smaller mandibles are less competitive and have lower fitness, and that fighting ability is genetically correlated with mandible size. Consistent with these findings, males evolving with male-predation have smaller mandibles and are less adept fighters.
Overall this study provides direct evidence that predator-mediated natural selection can evolutionarily reverse the exaggeration of a sexually selected trait. This finding is consistent with a vast body of fundamental theory and empirical evidence from observational and correlational studies (reviewed in the ref. ). We also reveal interesting outcomes when natural selection targets sex-limited sexually selected characters, since predator removal of a male-imposed conflict load increases female fitness. Thus sex-biased predation within a species can essentially mimic indirect ecological competition effects, as discussed above. Investigating the precise mechanistic detail and population level fitness effects of some of these findings is now required.
Methods
Gnatocerus stock culture
The G. cornutus beetle culture originated from adults collected in Miyazaki City (31° 54′N, 131° 25′E), Japan, and has been maintained in the laboratory of the National Food Research Institute, Japan, for about 50 years on whole meal enriched with yeast as food. The stock is made up of 1500–2000 beetles per generation and maintained in plastic cups (diameter 95 mm, height 50 mm) with a standing density of between 300 and 400 beetles per cup (for a more detailed description of the stock culture, see ref. ). This beetle is a stored product pest, and thus the laboratory conditions very closely mimic what have become their natural conditions. Virgin males and females were removed from the stock population as final instar larvae. Each larva was placed in one well of a 24-well tissue culture plate (Cellstar; Greiner Bio-One, Frickenhausen, Germany) until adult eclosion because pupation in G. cornutus is inhibited under high larval density. After eclosion, both sexes were allowed to sexually mature for a period of 14 days prior to their use. We performed all rearing and experiments in a chamber maintained at 25 °C, 60% relative humidity, and with a photoperiod cycle of 14:10 h light/dark. All experiments in this study follow this protocol unless stated otherwise.
The predator
The assassin bug Amphibolus venator is predator of stored-product insect pests and preys on various stored-product insect pests including flour beetles. These predators are frequently found in stored product facilities, which are the habitat of G. cornutus(and see Fig. 3). The A. venator culture originated from adults collected in a storehouse in Urasoe City, Okinawa, Japan, and has been maintained in the laboratory for about 5 years. The stock was initiated and maintained at 200 bugs per generation and housed in plastic containers (230 mm × 150 mm × 80 mm) with a standing density of between 30 and 40 bugs per cup. Each nymph was given an excess of food (seven final instar larvae of G. cornutus per week). Each adult female was allowed to mate with a male chosen randomly and to lay eggs in order to maintain the predator stocks. The predatory behavior of A. venator follows a stereotypical sequence: they recognize, chase, and capture prey (here G. cornutus) using their enlarged forelegs (similar to praying mantis)
Gnatocerus breeding design and estimation of quantitative genetic parameters
Using a full sib/half sib experimental design, males (sires) (N = 35) were randomly assigned to three virgin females (dams) (all collected from the stock population). Pairs were housed in a plastic container (17 mm diameter, 20 mm high) containing filter paper (17 mm diameter), and successful copulation was indicated by a stable end-to-end connection between the male and female. After mating, dams were immediately removed and individually placed in a plastic cup (70 mm diameter, 25 mm high) containing excess food (20 g). Each female was housed thus for two months to obtain offspring. All offspring from each female were reared to final instar (approximately 8 weeks). Three sons and three daughters per dam per sire (all pairings produced sufficient young) were haphazardly chosen for measurement of male traits (mandible, body, and abdomen size) and female traits (LRS, body, and abdomen size) at 14 days after eclosion (N = 315 per sex) (trait measurement protocols below). Although we were primarily interested in the genetics of male mandible size and female fitness, we included both abdomen size and total body size measures. This is because previous work found that directly selecting for larger mandibles caused a correlated decrease in abdomen size, but had no effect on total body size. This suggests a trade-off between abdomen and prothorax size and sexual conflict over body shape.
Data from the breeding design were then analysed using pedigree-based animal models fit in ASReml-R. First, to confirm the presence of additive genetic variance in each trait we fit a series of univariate animal models to (male) mandible, body and abdomen size, and to female (LRS). For each trait we compared the model fit to a reduced model with no additive genetic effects using a likelihood ratio test (LRT; adjusted for boundary conditions following). We elected to combine male and female records for both body size and abdomen sizes as additional modeling provided little support for genotype-by-sex interactions (see “Results” section). However, for these traits a fixed effect of sex was included, as well as the (random) additive genetic effect since exploratory analysis showed sexual dimorphism in both traits (body size, males are 0.020 mg (SE 0.005) larger on average, t = 3.58, P < 0.001; abdomen size, males are 0.019 mg (SE 0.008) smaller on average, t = 2.463, P = 0.014)).
We then fitted a multivariate animal model to estimate genetic correlation (rG) structure among the four traits with fixed effects of sex on body size, abdomen size, as well as their heritability (h2). The residual covariance structure was modeled as an unstructured matrix (but note residual covariance between the sex-limited traits of male mandible size and female LRS is not estimable from the data so was fixed to zero). We also ran reduced multivariate models with i) no genetic effects at all, and ii) a diagonal genetic variance matrix (i.e., genetic variance modeled on all traits but all genetic correlations assumed to equal zero) for comparison to the full model by LRT. This allows statistical inference at the level of the multivariate phenotype. We used estimated standard errors (SE) as a guide to nominal significance of pairwise genetic correlations (assuming approximate 95% CI are given by rG ± 1.96SE).
Gnatocerus experimental evolution protocol—sex specific predation
We first collected 900 male and 900 female G. cornutus from the stock culture and haphazardly generated nine groups of 100 males and 100 females to establish three male-predation populations, three female-predation populations and three control (no predation) populations (generation 0) (Fig. 5). To simulate predation, 100 males (or females) were housed in a plastic container (150 mm diameter, 50 mm high) containing an excess of beetle food (45 g). Then, five adult female A. venator (20–35 day olds) were randomly collected from the predator culture and placed into the container and the males (females) were exposed to them for two weeks. We then selected ten of the males (females) that survived the 2 weeks to act as sires (dams) of the predation treatments—ten opposite sex individuals were also taken/population to act as the non-selected dams (sires) that were not exposed to predation. We note that survival rate during this predation protocol was approximately 20%. To propagate control populations, ten males and ten females were haphazardly selected per population to act as sires and dams. For each population/treatment the ten males and females were placed in a plastic cup (diameter 95 mm, height 50 mm) with 70 g of medium for 2 months, with males able to mate with females and females were allowed to lay eggs, until final instar larvae were obtained. Final instar larvae were collected to obtain the adults for subsequent generations. When the adults reached 14 days old, 100 males and 100 females per population were randomly selected to (potentially) seed the next generation, and in the predation treatments, exposed to predators as above. We then selected surviving animals as above and repeated for eight generations. Additionally, we also collected 50 males and 50 females per population from generation 1 to 7 to assess mandible, abdomen and body size responses to selection. At generation 8, 20 males and 20 females per population were haphazardly collected for measurement of male traits (mandible, body, and abdomen size) and female traits (LRS, body and abdomen size) (N = 180 per sex) (trait measurement protocols below).
Fighting ability
We also conducted male–male fights between evolved males and males from stock populations (Fig. 5). Briefly, after generation eight of experimental evolution, males from the nine populations (control, male predation, and female predation) were collected. We observed 20 contests per population (n = 180 in total) in which focal experimental males competed against a male collected from the stock culture using standard methods. https://pagead2.googlesyndication.com/pagead/js/adsbygoogle.js In brief, the stock males were marked with white spots [Mitsubishi Paint-Marker] on their elytra for identification. Males 14 days old (after final eclosion) were used for the experiments. Before pairing, males were weighed with the electronic balance. To control for the effect of body size on fighting success, males were paired so that the difference in body size between contestants was less than 0.05 mg. Pairs were placed on filter-paper (17 mm diameter) in a plastic container (17 mm diameter, 20 mm high) and allowed to interact (and fight) for 1 h. Previous work has shown that male fights occur in all trials when staged in this manner. Males that pushed opponents and chased them were denoted the winner. Trials were then continuously observed until fight outcomes could be scored.
Predation and mandible size
To more directly test for possible associations between predation and mandible size, seventy adult female A. venator (20–35 days old) were randomly collected from the stock culture and were individually aspirated into in a plastic dish (90 mm diameter, 10 mm high) lined with a filter paper. After 30 min, one male adult G. cornutus collected from the stock culture (14 days old: mandible length measured prior to exposure) was added to each dish, and subsequent predation behavior was continuously observed for 30 min.
Male trait measurement
We measured overall body mass and the posterior body mass (i.e., mesothorax, metathorax, and abdomen) as an abdomen size indicator (and see comments above. Briefly, each male was frozen at −20 °C immediately after adult emergence. Mass measures were obtained to the nearest 0.01 mg on an electronic balance (Mettler-Toledo AG, Laboratory and Weighing Technologies). The mandible length (±0.01 mm) of each male was measured (±0.01 mm) using a dissecting microscope monitoring system (VM-60; Olympus, Tokyo, Japan). Each specimen was positioned so that its longitudinal and dorsoventral axes were perpendicular to the visual axes of the microscope eyepiece (see ref. for landmarks).
Female trait measurement
To obtain LRS (lifetime reproductive success: our fitness proxy) each female (14 days of post-eclosure) was individually paired with a haphazardly selected male from the stock culture. After copulation, each female was maintained in a plastic cup (70 mm diameter, 25 mm high) containing excess food (20 g) for two months and allowed to lay eggs. This schedule was chosen because most eggs are laid by females within 2 months of mating, and thus this is an accurate index of LRS. To measure the LRS of each female, we counted all adults that emerged in the third month after pairing. After the laying period, each female was frozen at −20 °C. Subsequently, the whole and posterior of the body (body size and abdomen size) were weighed with the electronic balance (as above).
Analysis
Apart from the genetic parameter estimation with an animal model (using ASReml-R as described above), all analyses were conducted using JMP for Windows version 8. For the experimental evolution, we used population as the unit of replication (=9 DF max.) with single fixed factor (with three levels: the experimental treatments) GLMs for each trait to test for effects of experimental evolution, with post-hoc testing for factor-level differences (note we did not have sufficient DF for a multivariate analysis). Results are as reported even after (conservative) sequential Bonferroni correction. Fighting results were compared (as population rates of winning) using a generalized linear model (GLM) (JMP 7). To directly assess how mandible size affected predation probability we used a GLM with a binomial distribution, a logit-link function, and overdispersion test. Male mandible size was used as the explanatory variable, and predation impact (preyed upon = 1, survive = 0) was the response variable.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are provided in Supplementary Data 1. This includes population mean trait values during and on completion of experimental evolution, fighting data, predation-mandible size data and the pedigree data. Source data are provided with this paper.
References
- 1.
Augmentation mammaire Tunisie
ReplyDeleteReduction mammaire Tunisie
Abdominoplastie Tunisie prix
ReplyDeleteLiposuccion Tunisie prix
Rhinoplastie Tunisie prix
Chirurgie générale prix
ReplyDeleteProthèse de hanche prix Tunisie
Prix sleeve Tunisie
عملية ترقيع غشاء البكارة في تونس
ReplyDeleteطفل الانبوب في تونس
التلقيح الاصطناعي في تونس